Saturday, September 14, 2019
Linguistic Research Essay
When does language begin? In the middle 1960s, under the influence of Chomskyââ¬â¢s vision of linguistics, the first child language researchers assumed that language begins when words (or morphemes) are combined. (The reading by Halliday has some illustrative citations concerning this narrow focus on ââ¬Å"structure. â⬠) So our story begins with what is colloquially known as the ââ¬Å"two-word stage. â⬠The transition to 2-word utterances has been called ââ¬Å"perhaps, the single most disputed issue in the study of language developmentâ⬠(Bloom, 1998). A few descriptive points: Typically children start to combine words when they are between 18 and 24 months of age. Around 30 months their utterances become more complex, as they add additional words and also affixes and other grammatical morphemes. These first word-combinations show a number of characteristics. First, they are systematically simpler than adult speech. For instance, function words are generally not used. Notice that the omission of inflections, such as -s, -ing, -ed, shows that the child is being systematic rather than copying. If they were simply imitating what they heard, there is no particular reason why these grammatical elements would be omitted. Conjunctions (and), articles (the, a), and prepositions (with) are omitted too. But is this because they require extra processing, which the child is not yet capable of? Or do they as yet convey nothing to the childââ¬âcan she find no use for them? Second, as utterances become more complex and inflections are added, we find the famous ââ¬Å"over-regularizationâ⬠ââ¬âwhich again shows, of course, that children are systematic, not simply copying what they here. Chomskyââ¬â¢s Influence Research on child language was behavioristic in the years that preceded Chomskyââ¬â¢s critique of Skinner, and his publication of Syntactic Structures: ââ¬Å"though there had been precedents for setting problems in the study of child language acquisition at a more abstract, cognitive level by continental scholarsââ¬âmost notably, Roman Jacobson (e. g. , 1941/1968)ââ¬âmuch of the research on child language acquisition at midcentury was influenced to a greater or lesser degree by the highly concrete, behaviorist orientation of B. F. Skinner and others. Two events were of major important in the change from behaviorist to cognitive thinking in research on child language. The first was Chomskyââ¬â¢s classic review (1959) of Verbal Behavior, Skinnerââ¬â¢s major book-length work on the learning and use of language; the second Handout for Psy 598-02, summer 2001 Packer Two-Word Utterances 2 was the detailed longitudinal study of the acquisition of English by three young children conducted over a 17-month period by Roger Brown and others in the early 1960s (Brown, 1973). â⬠Ritchie, W. C. , & Bhatia, T. K. (1999). Child language acquisition: Introduction, foundations, and overview. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds. ), Handbook of child language acquisition, (pp. 3-30). San Diego: Academic Press, p. 3-4 note 2. ââ¬Å"A child who has learned a language has developed an internal representation of a system of rulesâ⬠(Chomsky, 1965, p. 25). The psychologistââ¬â¢s task, it follows, is to determine what the childââ¬â¢s rules are. ââ¬Å"The linguist constructing a grammar for a language is in effect proposing a hypothesis concerning the internalized systemâ⬠(Chomsky, 1968, p.23). Up to the 1950s, people simply counted characteristics such as sentence complexity, proportion of grammatical utterances, etc. After Chomsky, the search was on for child grammars, assumed to be universal. Roger Brownââ¬â¢s Research In 1956 Roger Brown heard Chomsky for the first time, speaking at Yale. In 1962 he began a five-year research project on childrenââ¬â¢s language at Harvard University. The historical significance of Brownââ¬â¢s laboratory at Harvard can hardly be exaggerated. The names of students and colleagues who worked with Brown pop up all the time, to this day, in psycholinguistic research: the list includes Jean Berko Gleason, Ursula Bellugi, David McNeill, Dan Slobin, Courtney Cazden, Richard Cromer, Jill de Villiers, Michael Maratsos, Melissa Bowerman, Eleanor Rosche, Sue Ervin (now Ervin-Tripp), Steven Pinker. Brown set out to write grammars for each of the stages of language development, by looking at the distribution of forms and construction patterns in spontaneous speech. In most cases the data allow for more than oneà grammatical description. ââ¬Å"The description to be preferred, of course, is the one that corresponds to the way the speakerââ¬â¢s linguistic knowledge is structured, the one that determines the kinds of novel utterance he can produce or understand, how he constructs their meanings, and what his intuitions are about grammatical well-formednessâ⬠(Bowerman, 1988, p. 28) ââ¬Å"Every child processes the speech to which he is exposed so as to induce from it a latent structure. This latent rule structure is so general that a child can spin out its implications all his life longâ⬠¦. The discovery of latent structure is the greatest of the processes involved in language acquisition, and the most difficult to understandâ⬠(Brown & Bellugi, 1964, p. 314) Brown collected samples of spontaneous speech from three children, given the pseudonyms Adam, Eve, and Sarah. The corpus of collected data can be found in the Packer Two-Word Utterances 3 CHILDES archive. Eve was visited from age 18m to 26m, Adam from 27m to 42m, Sarah from 27m to 48m. Dan Slobin described the project: ââ¬Å"We paid close attention to the auxiliary system and to word-order patterns, because these had played a central role in Syntactic Structures. We kept track of sentence typesââ¬âaffirmative, negative, and questionsââ¬âin which use of auxiliaries and word order would vary. Linguistic growth was assessed in terms of things to be added to childish sentences to make them adult-like: the additions of omitted functors (inflections, prepositions, articles, and the like) and transformational operations. We did not categorize utterances in terms of communicative intentââ¬âthat is, in terms of semantics or speech acts or extended discourse skillsââ¬âand so we did not look for growth in terms of additions or enrichment of such abilities. Our central concern was with syntax and morphology, with some later interest in prosody. We worried about such questions as whether child grammar was finite state or transformational, and whether syntactic ââ¬Ëkernelsââ¬â¢ were the first sentence forms to appear in child speechâ⬠(Slobin, 1988, p. 11). Mean Length of Utterance This simple measure of syntactic complexity was introduced by Roger Brown. Table 7. Rules for calculating mean length of utterance and upper bound (Brown, 1973, p. 54) 1. Start with the second page of the transcription unless that page involves a recitation of some kind. In this latter case start with the first recitation-free stretch. Count the first100 utterances satisfying the following rules. 2. Only fully transcribed utterances are used; none with blanks. Portions of utterances, entered in parentheses to indicate doubtful transcription, are used. 3. Include all exact utterance repetitions (marked with a plus sign in records). Stuttering is marked as repeated efforts at a single word; count the word once in the most complete form produced. In the few cases where a word is produced for emphasis or the like (no, no, no) count each occurrence. 4. Do not count such fillers as mm or oh, but do count no, yeah, and hi. 5. All compound words (two or more free morphemes), proper names, and ritualized reduplications count as single words. Examples: birthday, rackety-boom, choo-choo, quack-quack, night-night, pocketbook, see saw. Justification is that no evidence that the constituent morphemes function as such for these children. 6. Count as one morpheme all irregular pasts of the verb (got, did, went, saw). Justification is that there is no evidence that the child relates these to present forms. 7.à Count as one morpheme all diminutives (doggie, mommie) because these children at least do not seem to use the suffix productively. Diminutives are the standard forms used by the child. 8. Count as separate morphemes all auxiliaries (is, have, will, can, must, would). Also all catenatives: gonna, wanna, hafta. These latter counted as single morphemes rather than as going to or want to because evidence is that they function so for the children. Count as separate morphemes all inflections, for example, possessive {s}, plural {s}, third person singular {s}, regular past {d}, progressive {ing}. 9. The range count follows the above rules but is always calculated for the total Packer Two-Word Utterances 4 transcription rather than for 100 utterances. The title of Brownââ¬â¢s 1973 book, summarizing of a decade of research (his own and other peopleââ¬â¢s), was A First Language: The Early Stages. A follow-up was planned, describing the ââ¬Å"laterâ⬠stages, but never written. What is this book about? ââ¬Å"It is about knowledge; knowledge concerning grammar and the meanings coded by grammarâ⬠¦. The book primarily presents evidence that knowledge of the kind described develops in an approximately invariant form in all children, through at different rates. There is also evidence that the primary determinants of the order are the relative semantical and grammatical complexityâ⬠(58) Here is an early attempt to write a ââ¬Å"syntacticâ⬠grammar of two-word speech, first describing only 89 observed utterances (Table 4), then going ââ¬Å"beyond the obtained sentences to the syntactic classes they suggest (Table 5) (Brown & Fraser, 1964, pp. 59, 61): Packer Two-Word Utterances 5 Brownââ¬â¢s Two Main Findings Two main findings are described in A First Language. 1. The ââ¬Å"Semantic Lookâ⬠of Stage I Speech First, that the organization of early word-combinations cannot be described in purely syntactic terms. Brown and his coworkers quickly had to change direction. Syntactic descriptions didnââ¬â¢t suffice. Thatââ¬â¢s to say, Stage I constructions couldnââ¬â¢t be satisfactorily explained either as ââ¬Å"telegraphicâ⬠speech, or in terms of ââ¬Å"pivot-openâ⬠grammar. Telegraphic Speech One of the first ways of characterizing 2-word utterances was to say that they omitted ââ¬Å"function words,â⬠such as articles, auxiliary verbs, inflexions, prepositions, and the copula (is). The words that are spoken tend to be nouns, verbs, and adjectives, and their order tends to resemble the order in what one presumes the adult sentence would be. These characteristics make early utterances sound like telegrams. But inflections are omitted too, and these are free in telegrams. And a few functors such as more, no, you and off are found. More important problems are that this description uses adult categories. And it doesnââ¬â¢t explain the productive character of childrenââ¬â¢s two-word utterances. Pivot-Open grammars Martin Braine suggested that children have simple rules they use to generate two-word utterances. Each pair of words selects one from a small set of wordsââ¬âcalled ââ¬Å"pivotsâ⬠ââ¬âthat occur in many utterances, and always in a fixed position (either the first word, or the second). For example, ââ¬Å"Allgoneâ⬠is a first-position pivot: allgone egg, allgone shoe, but not shoe allgone. A second-position pivot ââ¬Å"offâ⬠: shirt off, water off, etc. The choice of the second word is more ââ¬Å"open. â⬠Packer Two-Word Utterances 6 But ââ¬Å"the rules simply do not fit the evidence; pivot words do occur in isolation, pivots occur in combination with one another, sentences longer than two-words are fairly common in I, and there is distributional evidence which indicates that more than two word-classes existâ⬠(Brown, 1973, p. 110). Brown and his colleagues noted that adults ââ¬Å"expandâ⬠childrenââ¬â¢s utterances. These expansions donââ¬â¢t seem effective in teaching the child anything new (Cazden, 1965). But they do provide important clues to the researcher. If one assumes that adult expansions are generally accurate interpretations of the childââ¬â¢s utterance, then pivot-open grammars are inadequate because they underestimate the childââ¬â¢s knowledge. (Both would simply be described as O + O. ) For example, Lois Bloom showed that when one attended to context the utterance mommy sock was used by her child in two different ways. The first could be glossed as ââ¬Å"Itââ¬â¢s mommyââ¬â¢s sock,â⬠while the second could be glossed ââ¬Å"Mommy is putting on your sock. â⬠A pivot-open grammar would not be able to distinguish these two. From Non-Semantic (Lean) Grammars to Semantic (Rich) Grammars So Brown and his co-workers started instead to describe two-word utterances in semantic terms. They employed a process that Lois Bloom called ââ¬Å"rich interpretationâ⬠: using all the contextual information available to infer what the child meant by an utterance. As Lois Bloom said, ââ¬Å"evaluation of the childrenââ¬â¢s language began with the basic assumption that it was possible to reach the semantics of childrenââ¬â¢s sentences by considering nonlinguistic information from context and behavior in relation to linguistic performance. This is not to say that the inherent ââ¬Ëmeaningââ¬â¢ or the childââ¬â¢s actual semantic intent was obtainable for any given utterance. The semantic interpretation inherent in an utterance is part of the intuition of the child and cannot be ââ¬Ëknownââ¬â¢ with authority. The only claim that could be made was the evaluation of an utterance in relation to the context in which it occurred provided more information for analyzing intrinsic structure than would a simple distributional analysis of the recorded corpusâ⬠(Bloom, 1970, p. 10). The result was the identification of a small set of basic semantic relations that the childrenââ¬â¢s utterances seems to be expressing. The eight most common of these are summarized in the following table (cf. Brown, p.193-197): ââ¬Å"Major Meanings at Stage Iâ⬠Two-Word Utterance mommy come; daddy sit drive car; eat grape mommy sock; baby book go park; sit chair cup table; toy floor my teddy; mommy dress Semantic relation expressed agent + action action + object agent + object action + location entity + location possessor + possession Packer Two-Word Utterances 7 box shiny; crayon big dat money; dis telephone entity + attribute demonstrative + entity It seems that children when they first combine words talk about objects: pointing them out, naming them, indicating their location, what they areà like, who owns them, and who is doing things to them. They also talk about actions performed by people, and the objects and locations of these actions. Brown suggested that these are the concepts the child has just finished differentiating in the sensorimotor stage. This kind of semantic characterization of childrenââ¬â¢s speech continues in current research. For example, the following table is redrawn from Golinkoff & Hirsh-Pasek, (1999, p. 151. ) The terminology differs a little, and Recurrence and Disappearance have been added (or at least were not in Brownââ¬â¢s ââ¬Å"top eightâ⬠), but other than this the picture is the same. Two-Word Utterance Mommy sock Probable meaning expressed Possessor-possessed or Agent (acting on) an object Recurrence Disappearance or Nonexistence Action on object Agent doing an action Object at location Object and property Naming Possible gloss ââ¬Å"Thatââ¬â¢s Mommyââ¬â¢s sockâ⬠or ââ¬Å"Mommy, put on my sockâ⬠ââ¬Å"I want more juiceâ⬠ââ¬Å"The outside is allgoneâ⬠(said after front door is closed) ââ¬Å"(Dad) is throwing the toy chickenâ⬠ââ¬Å"The car is goingâ⬠ââ¬Å"The sweater is on the chairâ⬠ââ¬Å"The dog is littleâ⬠ââ¬Å"That is Susanâ⬠or ââ¬Å"Her name is Susanâ⬠. More juice! Allgone outside Throw chicken Car go Sweater chair Little dog That Susan What Grammar to Write? How to represent the knowledge that underlies childrenââ¬â¢s utterances viewed in these semantic terms? What kind of grammar can one write? Brown (1973) reviewed several possibilities are concluded that ââ¬Å"No fully explicit grammar proves to be possibleâ⬠(p. 244). Bloom wrote essentially syntactic grammars, which however included information necessary to give an appropriate semantic interpretation. Schlesinger (assigned reading) wrote a semantic grammar. Antinucci & Paresi (optional reading) wrote a grammar that included some pragmatic information too. The following is a grammar for one of the three children Bloom studied: it ââ¬Å"consists of (1) the phrase structure, (2) lexico feature rules, and (3) transformations (Bloom, 1970, pp. 67-68): Packer Two-Word Utterances 8 Packer Two-Word Utterances 9 Criticism of Interpretive Analysis An interesting criticism of these semantic analyses was made by Howe in 1976. Howe noticed a lack of consistency across semantic categorization of two-word utterances by Bloom, Slobin, Schlesinger and Brown, and suggested that the identification of semantic relations actually tells us more about adult interpretation of childrenââ¬â¢s speech that is does about what the child has in mind. ââ¬Å"Overall, the existence of contradictions between the categories presented in Table 1, the fact that some of the categories are not always mutually exclusive and the fact that it is hard to demonstrate that some of the so-called ââ¬Ësemanticââ¬â¢ distinctions are more than syntactic alternatives for expressing the same meaning, make it unlikely that Bloom, Brown, Schlesinger and Slobin have produced an adequate categorization of the meanings common to the speech of children at the beginnings of word combination or indeed of adultsâ⬠¦. [A]ll four writers tacitly assumed that the two-word utterances of young children always express a meaning adults might express using these words and hence their aim was to specify which of the meanings adults might express occur in the first word combinationsâ⬠(Howe, 1976, p. 34). Howe asserted that (as she later put it) ââ¬Å"there was no evidence that children at the beginning of word combination recognize a world containing agents, locations, and so onâ⬠(Howe, 1981, p. 443). It is interesting to read the next rounds of this debate: Bloom, Capatides, & Tackeff (1981), Golinkoff (1981), and Howeââ¬â¢s reply (1981). Bloom is witheringly derisive (and seems to miss the point of Howeââ¬â¢s article), Golinkoff is more constructive. Howe accepts Golinkoffââ¬â¢s suggestion that non-linguistic data will show us how a child understands their situation, and she concludes that so far the research shows ââ¬Å"that children do not discover that language encodes roles [played in actions and states of affairs, as distinct from entities involved in actions and states of affairs], until some time after their first word combinationsâ⬠(451). But Ià think thereââ¬â¢s a larger point here that Iââ¬â¢ll explore in class. Brownââ¬â¢s conclusions about Stage I Brown drew the following conclusions about Stage I: ââ¬Å"The Stage I child operates as if all major sentence constituents were optional, and this does not seem to be because of some absolute ceiling on sentence complexity. In Stage II and after we shall see that he operates, often for long periods, as if grammatical morphemes were optional. Furthermore, the childââ¬â¢s omissions are by no means limited to the relatively lawful omissions which also occur in adult speech. He often leaves out what is linguistically obligatory. This suggests to me that the child expects always to be understood if he produces any appropriate words at all. And in fact we find that he would usually be right in this expectation as long as he speaks at home, in familiar surroundings, and to family members who know his history and inclinations. Stage I speech may then be said to be well adapted to its communicative purpose, well adapted but narrowly adapted. In new surroundings and with less familiar addresses it wouldà Packer Two-Word Utterances 10 often fail. This suggests that a major dimension of linguistic development is learning to express always and automatically certain things (agent, action, number, tense, and so on) even though these meanings may be in many particular contexts quite redundant. The child who is going to move out into the world, as children do, must learn to make his speech broadly and flexible adaptiveâ⬠(Brown, 1973, p. 244-245). 2. The Acquisition of Grammatical Morphemes in Stage IIà The second major finding that Brown reported in A First Language was that ââ¬Å"a set of little words and inflections begins to appear: a few prepositions, especially in and on, an occasional article, an occasional copula am, is, or are, the plural and possessive inflections on the noun, the progressive, past, and third person present indicative inflections on the verb. All these, like an intricate sort of ivy, begin to grow up between and upon the major construction blocks, the nouns and the verbs, to which Stage I is largely limitedâ⬠(Brown, 1973, p.à 249). Brown found that the 14 of these grammatical morphemes of English that he selected for detailed study were acquired in a fixed and universal order. These are the grammatical morphemes we discussed in an earlier class: affixes like ââ¬âs, -ed, {PAST}, and small function words like on, in, the. Weââ¬â¢ve already noted that these morphemes are omitted from the first word-combinations. Brown studied the way they are gradually added to a childââ¬â¢s speech. This takes place in what he called Stage II. The child begins to explicitly mark notions such as number, specificity, tense, aspect, mood, using the inflections or unbound morphemes. Of course, Brown was studying only three children, but the finding of invariant order has stood up when larger numbers of children have been studied. For example, de Villiers and de Villiers (1973) replicated his finding with a sample of twenty-one children. Brown offered evidence that the order of their acquisition was determined by their linguistic complexity. (Thatââ¬â¢s to say, the number of features each of them encoded.) (Though he noted too that children differ greatly in their rate of acquisition of these morphemes. ) Order 1. 2/3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. Morpheme present progressive prepositions plural irregular past tense possessive copula uncontractible articles regular past tense third-person present tense regular Example singing; playing in the cup; on the floor books; dolls broke; went Mommyââ¬â¢s chair; Susieââ¬â¢s teddy This is my book The teddy; A table walked; played he climbs; Mommy cooks Packer Two-Word Utterances 11 11. 12. 13. 14.à third-person present tense irregular auxiliary uncontractible copula contractible auxiliary contractible John has three cookies She was going to school; Do you like me? Iââ¬â¢m happy; you are special Mommyââ¬â¢s going shopping Brown examined each utterance is see whether it required any of these morphemes to make it fully grammatical by adult standards, attending to both linguistic and nonlinguistic context. E. g. , when the child points to a book and says that book, Brown inferred that there should have been a copula (ââ¬Ës or is) and an article (a). Then he checked how many of these obligatory positions for each morpheme were actually filled with the appropriate morphemes at each age. Acquisitionââ¬âdefined as the age at which a morpheme is supplied in 90 percent of its obligatory positionsââ¬âwas remarkably constant across Brownââ¬â¢s three subjects. Why did Brown study these morphemes? Presumably because they are at first omitted. But more importantly, he was trying to test the hypothesis that children are taught grammar by adults. And Brown found that frequency of exposure (in adult speech) was not a predictor. For example, adults used articles more frequently than prepositions, but children acquired these in the opposite order. Brown suggested that linguistic complexity does predict acquisition. The morphemes differ in both semantic complexity (the number of semantic features encoded) and syntactic complexity (the number of rules each requires). For example, the copula verb encodes both number and temporality. These two types of complexity are highly correlated, so they cannot be teased apart, but in either case they predict order of acquisition. The other important change that occurs in Stage II is that, as utterances grow in complexity, the child begins to combine two or more of the basic semantic relations from Stage I: Adam hit ball = agent + action + object = agent + action, plus action + object The Other Stages of Language Acquisition Each of the five stages that Brown distinguished is named for the linguistic process that is the major new development occurring in that stage (ââ¬Å"or for an exceptionally elaborate development of a process at that stageâ⬠p. 59). Thus we have: Packer Two-Word Utterances 12. Stage I. Semantic Roles & Syntactic Relations. MLU: 1. 0 ââ¬â 2. 0 agent, patient, instrument, locative etc. expressed (in simple sentences) by linear order, syntactic relations, prepositions or postpositions. Stage II. Grammatical Morphemes & the Modulation of Meaning. MLU: 2. 0 ââ¬â 2. 5 Stage III. Modalities of the Simple Sentence. MLU: 2. 5 Next the child forms transformations of simple declarative sentences: yes-no interrogatives, question request, negation, imperative. During the earlier stages children use intonation to mark different sentence modalities. Now they begin to use morphosemantic devices to mark negatives, questions, and imperatives. Stage IV. Embedding of Sentences One simple sentence will now become used as a grammatical constituent or in a semantic role within another sentence. Stage V. Coordination of Simple Sentences & Propositional Relations Sentences are linked together with connector words. Individual Differences Brown also noted some individual differences among Adam, Eve, and Sarah. Two of the children combined V with N, and also used N for possession: eat meat, throw ball, mommy sock. But the child third combined V (or objects of possession) with pronouns: eat it, do this one, my teddy. These two strategies were found by other researchers too. Catherine Nelson called them pronominal & nominal strategies (they have also been called ââ¬Å"holistic & analyticâ⬠; ââ¬Å"expressive & referentialâ⬠), and noted that they could be seen in one-word utterances also: some children tend to produce single-word utterances that are nouns, other children tend to use social or personal words such as hi, bye, and please. Subsequent research has explored the connections between these strategies and later development, cognitive style, and input differences (cf. Shore, 1995. Individual differences in language development, Sage). However, these strategies converge over time. By MLU=2. 5, sentence subjects (agents) are typically pronominal, and predicate objects (patients) are typically nominal. Packer Two-Word Utterances 13 Directions After Brown By the mid-1970s grammar-writing was dying out. Incorrect predictions had discouraged researchers, as had the problem of indeterminacy: the fact that more than one grammar could be written. Interest was growing in other considerations: in the role of semantics; in cognitive precursors to syntax, and to language in general; in mother-child interaction; and in the pragmatic uses to which early speech is put. In the view of some people, linguistic structures and operations became neglected. 1. How Does the Child go from Semantics to Syntax? Weââ¬â¢ve seen that Brownââ¬â¢s research found that the grammar of childrenââ¬â¢s early word combinations was better described in semantic than in syntactic terms. If this is so, how does a child make the transition from a semantic grammar to the adult grammar? Researchers continue to argue about this. Steven Pinker (1984, 1987) suggests that children use semantics to enter the syntactic system of their language. In simple ââ¬Å"basic sentencesâ⬠the correspondence between things and names maps onto the syntactic category of nouns. Words for physical attributes and changes of state map onto verbs. Semantic agents are almost always the grammatical subjects of sentences. This semantic-syntactic correspondence in early utterances provides a key to abstract syntactic categories of grammar. Paul Bloom has argued that children actually are using syntactic categories from the start, and he cites as evidence for this the fact that children will they place adjectives before nouns but not pronouns: big dog but not: * small she Some linguists have offered a syntactic description of Stage I utterances. They argue that at this stage children merely have a lexicon and a limited set of phrase structure rules in deep-structure. They lack functional categories such as INFL (inflectionals) and COMP (complementizers). No transformations exist at this stage: instead, elements of the deep structure are assigned thematic (i. e. semantic) roles to yield the surfacestructure. And they have proposed that the lack of grammatical subjects in Stage I utterances reflects the default setting of a ââ¬Å"null-subject parameter. â⬠(Since in languages like Italian and Spanish a subject is optional. ) Lois Bloom (1990b) has suggested that children simply have a more limited processing capacity at this age. Sentence subjects are often provided by context, and so can be safely omitted. Dan Slobin has proposed that ââ¬Å"children create grammars in which clearly identifiable surface forms map onto basic semantic categoriesâ⬠(1988, p. 15). Packer Two-Word Utterances 14 For example, locative prepositionsââ¬âin, on, underââ¬âare omitted in early child speech. They are used earlier in languages when they are encoded more salientlyââ¬âas noun suffixes or as postpositions following nouns. At the same time, there is a common order of emergence across languages: simple topological notions of proximity, containment and support (in, on, under, next to), with locative relations embodying notions of perspective (back, front) always later. Slobin infers that ââ¬Å"conceptual development provides the content for linguistic expression, while linguistic discovery procedures are necessary for working out the mapping of content according to conventions of particular languagesâ⬠(p. 15). Slobin has looked carefully at the English grammatical morphemesââ¬âand their equivalents in other languagesââ¬âto see how they are used before they are completely acquired (by Brownââ¬â¢s 90% criterion). He finds that children generally use the morphemes systematically, though their use is still ââ¬Å"incompleteâ⬠by adult standards. For example, a Russian child applied the accusative inflection only to nouns that ââ¬Å"were objects of direct, physical manipulation, such as ââ¬Ëgive,ââ¬â¢ ââ¬Ëcarry,ââ¬â¢ ââ¬Ëput,ââ¬â¢ and ââ¬Ëthrow,ââ¬â¢ omitting the accusative for less manipulative verbs such as ââ¬Ëreadââ¬â¢ and ââ¬Ësee. ââ¬â¢Ã¢â¬ Children will ââ¬Å"organize systems of pronouns and case inflections; but, to begin with, children will organize these various forms to express particular, child-oriented speech functionsâ⬠(p. 18). They are using the resources of the adult language to mark distinctions that are salient to them. Slobin has also proposed some ââ¬Å"universal language-learning principles. â⬠These are an attempt to explain observed cross-language regularities in order of acquisition. ââ¬Å"According to Slobin, the child has certain concepts, based on cognitive growth, that are expressed through the language system. Using certain principles of acquisition, the child scans the language code to discover the means of comprehension and productionâ⬠(Owens, 2001, p. 214-215). 1. Pay attention to the ends of words 2. Phonological forms of words can be systematically modified 3. Pay attention to the order of words and morphemes 4. Avoid interruption and rearrangement of linguistic units 5. Underlying semantic relations should be marked overtly and clearly 6. Avoid exceptions 7. The use of grammatical markers should make semantic sense Knowledge of Verb syntax Lois Bloom asserts that learning the argument structure of verbs, and the syntactic differences for different thematic relations is the foundation for acquiring a grammar. Verbs play a central role in further multiword utterances. Opinions differ, however, on how knowledge of verb syntax is acquired. Bloom suggests that the first verbs are those that name actions (do, make, push, eat). Nouns and pronouns take thematic roles (agent, object) in relation to these actions. Bloom says that this implies that childrenââ¬â¢s ââ¬Å"theoriesâ⬠of objects, space, and causation are important here. Packer Two-Word Utterances 15 A few all-purpose verbsââ¬âââ¬Å"pro-verbsâ⬠ââ¬âare used for most early sentences. E. g. , do, go. With these, verb argument structures, verb inflections, and Wh-questions are learned. Subsequently, the child adds the syntax for negation, noun- and verb-inflection, and questions. And then moves on to embedded verb phrases (ââ¬Å"drink [Mommy juice]â⬠) 2. From Semantics to Semantics Language involves a great deal of categorization. ââ¬Å"The forms of language are themselves categories, and these forms are linked to a vast network of categorical distinctions in meaning and discourse functionâ⬠(Bowerman, 1988, p. 28-29).
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.